Have a look at this infographic and see if you can be persuaded to take a nap- it’s not just good for students!
Click image to view PDF or read below.
Do you think you can fit naps into your daily life? Let me know why/not!
Have a look at this infographic and see if you can be persuaded to take a nap- it’s not just good for students!
Click image to view PDF or read below.
Do you think you can fit naps into your daily life? Let me know why/not!
Nooit Meer Slapen (No More Sleep)
A tv-documentary about living in a 24-hour economy, and its impact on our sleep pattern (in Dutch).
At first, it looks like 24/7 economy negatively impacts our quality and duration of sleep. We can stay up late because there is always something to do, and when we are finally in bed we keep going on our electronic devices. A big part of us feeling like that is a problem, could be the dogma of having to sleep in a solid, 8-hour block each night. What if we would return to a more natural pattern of segmented sleep? This could consist of one late-night sleep, and a good nap in the afternoon. After all, it is not the duration of sleep that counts most for cognitive and physical abilities, it is the quality of sleep that matters.
Does 24/7 economy help the return of natural segmented sleep? Perhaps if we can create circumstances where sleeping is also possible 24/7, we can do it all and still feel rested.
This article describes the two techniques of measuring when a person is asleep: actigraphy and polysomnography.
Polysomnography (PSG) monitors many body functions including brain (EEG), eye movements (EOG), muscle activity or skeletal muscle activation (EMG) and heart rhythm (ECG) during sleep. After the identification of the sleep disorder sleep apnea in the 1970s, the breathing functions respiratory airflow and respiratory effort indicators were added along with peripheral pulse oximetry.
Actigraphy is done by a portable device usually worn around the wrist or ankle that registers movement. Actigraphy is much less cumbersome and invasive, and therefore more suitable to naturalistic and long term experiments.
The present study aims to determine if actigraphy can detect accurately sleep in healthy, young adults during a 90-min mid-afternoon nap opportunity when compared to PSG.
Conclusion:
actigraphy was fairly proficient in distinguishing the difference between a nap and a no-nap period of quiet rest. Although actigraphy overestimated sleep during the no-nap condition, the accuracy values remained reasonably high
Nurtureshock: Why Everything We Thought About Children is Wrong – Po Bronson and Ashley Merryman
This review is only about Chapter 2, since that is about sleep, which is the subject of my current project. The other chapters are equally interesting though! In the chapter ‘The Lost Hour’, the authors describe how children today sleep one hour less than they did 30 years ago and how that impacts their wellbeing.
Like in several scientific papers I read on this subject, it is stated here that a high percentage of high-schoolers feel sleepy during school, their grades drop because of that, and some fall asleep in class. They go as far as to say that the average amount of sleep a child in high school gets, is only 6,5 hours!
The causes for this lack of sleep are the usual suspects: full schedules, early school start times, adolescent circadian clock shifts. Not much time is spend describing possible solutions for this lack of sleep, other than that teenagers who went to bed earlier, got more sleep; their IQ went up and so did their grades. But how to motivate teenagers to go to bed early?
Then an interesting part about adolescents. For reference, I looked up the definition of adolescence in several sources. I was disappointed to find no watertight definition of it, other than some rather vague descriptions without much overlap. The World Health Organisation defines adolescence as the period between 10 and 20 years of age. In the US, adolescent age is often defined as between 13 and 24.
Brown’s Mary Carskadon has demonstrated that during puberty, the circadian system – the biological clock – does a ‘phase shift’ that keeps adolescents up later. In prepubescents and grownups, when it gets dark outside, the brain produces melatonin, which makes us sleepy, But adolescent brains don’t release melatonin for another 90 minutes. So even if teenagers are in bed at 10 p.m. (which they aren’t), they lie awake, staring at the ceiling.
Several experiments with pushing back school start times and letting teens sleep longer, have yielded better results on SAT tests. And interestingly, also typical modern puberescent behaviours like moodiness, depression and binge eating go down with more sleep. Could it be that todays teenagers act out more because of their lack of sleep? The authors of Nurtureshock certainly believe so.
Another side effect of sleep deprivation is an increased chance of becoming obese. Hormone levels change with sleep loss, increasing the feeling of hunger and favouring storage of fat. Furthermore, when you sleep less, you are tired during the day, so you are less likely to have an active lifestyle on top of being hungry. Plus, when you are asleep, you are not eating!
For my research, physical health could be an interesting aspect to define benefits of the proposed nap with. Next to factors like daytime sleepiness, moodiness and cognitive abilities, physical health is important. It would be good to incorporate some measure of that, be it weight, blood pressure or resting heart rate.
This paper is one of the proceeds of a symposium held by the authors (“The Effect of Naps on Health and Cognition”, held at the 5th Congress of the World Federation of Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine Societies in Cairns, Australia, September 2007). It describes several research questions and nap-hypotheses, and an agenda for further research on naps is proposed.
Practice points:
Research agenda:
Especially the second to last point is of interest to me. Would ‘younger children’ also include adolescents though?
Through the use of the Chronic Sleep Reduction Questionnaire (CSRQ), the researchers try to assess symptoms of chronic sleep reduction in adolescents. While this paper is mostly about validating this approach, the information is very useful to see how such experiments are set up, and their conclusions. Also the questionnaire itself can be a useful tool in further research. Continue reading
An interesting article comparing three methods of reducing daytime sleepiness against a control group.
The setup was to test one countermeasure each week, in one test day a week, to see the effects of:
Functional Anatomy of the Sleep-Wakefulness Cycle
This book is mainly about the specific nerves, hormones and other body parts responsible for sleep. Of course I should have known from the title, which has ‘anatomy’ in it, but this particular book was not too helpful for me. In a big part due to the fact I could not really read about the various experiments without gagging. But I concentrated and read through at least one particularly creative experiment.
The Sleep-Wakefulness-Cycle (SWC) simplified, exists of three stages: wake, non-REM sleep, REM sleep. Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep, is when you dream. Apparently, the cycle becomes longer in time, later in the night, which could explain why most people remember more complex dreams in the morning compared with when they wake up in the middle of the night. Also REM sleep is diminished in older people.
An experiment is described to see whether input on the body, influences the SWC. Normally, darkness causes certain hormones to be released which is why people and many animals sleep at night and are awake at day. The brain gives the command to release the hormones. But does the animal ‘know’ that its night, through his body or brain?
The conclusion was, that influences on the spinal cord nerves, must be responsible for the circadian rhythm. In other words, our brain does not dictate when we sleep, our body does!
Until now, I have read research, newspaper articles and books on sleep in general, sleeping adolescents, and naps. I have discovered that:
I would like to find out more about the possibilities to fight sleepiness in adolescents with the use of naps, but I have not been able to find many scientific papers on that subject. Perhaps this is a good area to do more research in. In order to study the effects of a nap on teenagers, I need to find out or define:
During the project, I will update this post and work towards a good research setup. In the mean time, if you want to read more on sleep: check out this post
From the New York Times: Rethinking sleep by David K. Randall
Mr. Randall states that we only started believing in 8-solid-hours-sleep in Western society, and only after the industrial revolution. Perhaps midday naps were unpopular to factory owners? There are other unhealthy sleeping habits connected to the industrialized world: think of shift work and night jobs.
But even today, in many societies naps are a way of life. In Spain, although in decline, people take a siesta after lunch, the main meal of the day. In China and other Asian countries, taking a powernap at your desk makes you seem motivated and effective, not lazy.
Further in the article, texts from historical records like Shakespeare are cited as proof that daytime sleeping was once common.
A character in the “Canterbury Tales,” for instance, decides to go back to bed after her “firste sleep.” A doctor in England wrote that the time between the “first sleep” and the “second sleep” was the best time for study and reflection. Continue reading